SLA Topics SLA Bibliography Vivian Cook
The Neglected Role of Written Language in Language Teaching
Vivian Cook
(the basis for a chapter in L2 Writing Systems, 2005)
Since the decline of audiolingualism, there has been little public debate about the respective roles of spoken language and written language in language teaching. This paper argues that it is time to start thinking about this relationship again. The basic question it asks is how teaching should reflect speech and writing in the early stages of language learning, concentrating on the beginners level rather than more advanced levels of writing, which have received more attention.
1. Teaching Background
The priority of speech has been a constant theme in teaching methodology (Banathy & Sawyer, 1969), forming article 1 of the International Phonetics Association in the 1880s 'Foreign language study should begin with the spoken language of everyday life' (cited in Stern, 1983). The audiolingualism of the 1960s took the first principle of ‘scientific language teaching’ as ‘Speech before writing’ (Lado, 1964). The only twentieth century exception was perhaps the short-lived Direct Reading Method (Coleman, 1929).
The bias towards speech has not altered with the decline of audiolingualism. The Natural Approach to language teaching emphasises ‘the ability to understand and speak’ (Terrell, Egasse & Andrade, 1990, p.xix). The English curriculum in Cuba insists on 'The principle of the primacy of spoken language' (Cuban Ministry of Education, 1999). Communicative language teaching uses ‘class-room activities designed to get learners to speak and listen to each other’ (Scrivenor, 1994, p.62). Alternative methods such as Total Physical Response (TPR) use story-telling, not story-reading (Seely & Romijn, 1995). Few people have taken the opposing view that written language should be the basis of second language (L2) teaching, except, perhaps, for Krashen's advocacy of Free Voluntary Reading as a form of comprehensible input (Krashen, 1993). In 1984 Howatt claimed ‘The spoken language for example is promoted with more determination now than at any time since the Reform Movement’ (Howatt, 1984, p.289), which seems as true two decades later.
As always, the discussion is clouded by the gap between 'official' language methodology and the reality in the classroom; what relationship do the recommendations by methodologists, syllabuses etc have to what actually goes on in the classroom? That is to say, even if teachers are exhorted to use the spoken language, do they really do so? The bridge that is used here is course-books; if teachers' practice departed too much from the coursebook, then presumably it would not be on sale for very long.
2. the Use of Written Language in Beginner's Course-books
To show how written language is handled in teaching, let us look at some beginners' course-books available for international markets, not just for English but across other languages, namely seven adult low-level courses: five for English – Atlas (Nunan, 1995), Reward (Greenall, 1994), Changes (Richards 1998), Tapestry 1 Listening and Speaking (Benz & Dworak, 2000), Headway Elementary (Soars & Soars, 1993) – and two for other languages – Ci Siamo (Guarnaccio & Guarnaccio, 1997) for Italian and Libre Echange (Courtillon & de Salins, 1995) for French. Changes, Libre Echange and Atlas claim to be suitable for beginners, Reward for 'intermediate', Headway for 'elementary' and Tapestry for 'high beginning'; Ci Siamo does not specify level. All the courses apart from Ci Siamo are monolingual and all except for Tapestry are stand-alone volumes. These are believed to be representative of 1990s language teaching materials. The usual convention will be adopted here of angle brackets for written forms such as <bl>, parallel to the use of slants for phonetic script /bl/, and italics for examples such as table.
a) written scripts of spoken dialogues or monologues
Written transcripts of spoken language form an essential component in most courses, usually highly scripted: Cameriere: Buonasera. S'accomodi. Ecco il menù. …(Ci Siamo). Tapestry 1, Reward and Headway Elementary provide scripts of the listening materials at the back of the book. Changes has two short scripted conversations in every unit. Ci Siamo uses techniques from the teen magazines of photo-stories with captions and speech-balloons. Most scripts in course-books resemble plays. Indeed Libre Echange uses authentic film scripts from inter alia La Dentellière and Pauline à la plage. Though the early Reform Movement valued phonetic script as a way of showing spoken language on the page (Sweet, 1899), this practice survives only in the phonetic symbols used in some vocabulary lists (Headway) and sparingly in pronunciation practice in Libre Echange and Reward.
The use of scripts shows written language in the service of speech. The written text provides a permanent form of the speech the students encounter that can be referred to, analysed and revised. Scripts are not teaching writing since the characteristics are all supposedly those of spoken language. They provide incidental exposure to the characteristics of written language but in an arbitrary and unplanned way; for instance leçon <<zéro>> of Libre Echange introduces three conventions of written French in its title (<ç>, <é> and 'goosefeet' quotation marks << >>). The reverse concept of written language to be read aloud is never employed in these coursebooks, even if familiar from news-readers or priests reading texts aloud.
b) written components of teaching
language explanations
Written descriptions of pronunciation or grammar are given in the coursebooks, sometimes in the L1: Regular verbs ending in -ire are divided into two groups … (Ci Siamo), though mostly in the L2: Le «u» de «tu» tombe devant une voyelle (Libre Echange), extended to learning strategies in Tapestry 1: Use music to help you learn English and to improve your pronunciation. Related uses are displays of grammar in substitution tables and word-sorting: Write the words in the correct columns. Wednesday night Friday singer … (Atlas). Many courses display structures in columns as the main means of 'explaining' grammar, whether 'going to' future or 'have got' (Changes), as described in Cook, (1989).
Written explanations again exploit the permanency of written language, columns its visual display properties. Perhaps only in a coursebook is grammar explained in this way. (And furthermore they embody a pre-1957 view of phrase structure (Cook,, 1989)).
conveying meaning
Written language conveys the meaning of unknown words, sometimes in the L2, sometimes in the L1. One possibility is to label pictures – parts of the anatomy of Michelangelo's David: Il corpo 1) i capelli 2) la fronte…: (Ci Siamo) or: Write the name of the correct symptom under each picture…: a fever, a stuffy nose, a headache … (Tapestry). Another is lists of translation equivalents; Al Bar. il caffè coffee il cameriere waiter … . (Ci Siamo). This overlaps with the practice of matching pictures with descriptions: Match a picture with an activity. Dancing, visiting museums, doing crosswords, …: (Headway), or of identifying elements in a photo: Which of the things can you see in the photo? Apples, bananas, beef, beer … (Reward).
Outside coursebooks, diagrams are used in manuals for car maintenance or drawings of cooking utensils, in which the point of the labelling is to aid the activity; it doesn't matter whether you know what a gudgeon pin is the day after servicing your car provided you have oiled it correctly. In course-books, however, the point of the diagram is the link between the image and the new word.
giving instructions about exercises
Instructions for exercises are given in writing, sometimes in the L2: Comparez les differents formulations (Libre Echange), sometimes in the L1: Interview a classmate and find out how they spent last weekend (Ci Siamo); Changes gives the script for a Total Physical Response sequence (a) Close your book, please. (b) Open your exercise book, please. (c) …. , illustrated by drawings of the actions. Possibly such instructions are more for the teacher than for the students as their language is often way above the students' level: Lesson 1 of Atlas for example has: Match the countries and nationalities and then say the words, involving not only vocabulary which has not been taught, for example words, but also imperatives match/say and structures co-ordinated by and – Match … and … say.
Outside the classroom instructions are a restricted genre of written language. Written instructions mostly tell individuals how to do a tasks such as filling in their income tax return or baking a cake, not how to interact with a partner or a group of people in the manner of most classroom instructions.
asking comprehension questions
The text of the spoken conversations is often followed by written questions checking comprehension: Fabienne, Carmen et Eric sortent: ou vont-ils? (Libre Echange). The answers may be ticks: Who owns these things? Listen and tick the right name (Changes), multiple choice questions(Tapestry), matching complaints with the advice given by a doctor (Reward), or filling in a chart such as: Listen to four people talking about the food they eat on different days of the week. Complete the chart. (Changes).
Once again written questions about what you have heard orally would rarely occur outside a classroom. However realistic the spoken language that students hear, subsequent comprehension questions are not a typical use of written language.
c) providing exercise props
Many spoken exercises rely on fragments of written language as props: could the communicative method indeed exist without written language prompts?
lists.
Often these props are lists of words, for instance: Match the foods and drinks to the words in the chart (Changes). Sometimes they are prompts for ideas: Computers. Can they ….? Count smell translate … (Headway), or for sentences: Make a sentence with each of these weather words … rain dry sunny … (Tapestry). A variant is sorting word lists into columns or charts. Reward makes students sort ballet cinema film interval ticket into What's on?, Where and Related words.
Little outside the educational context provides a model or a purpose for these isolated pieces of written language. The only parallel might be making a speech from notes, which is self-prepared and far more complex.
realia
Information necessary for the exercise can be conveyed through realia and graphics, such as the labelling on charts, horoscopes, calendars – both Ci Siamo and Libre Echange have saints' calendars – shopping catalogues: i jeans 1 avoria L.50.00, 2 nero L.79.00 … (Ci Siamo), and ephemera such as supermarket fliers (Atlas, Ci Siamo). Maps are particularly popular whether San Francisco (Atlas), West Valley College Saratoga (Tapestry), London (Changes), Dublin (Reward) or Urbania (Ci Siamo). Occasionally the coursebooks provide actual statistical charts, for example, regions of Italy popular with tourists (Ci Siamo), percentage of U.S. population that was foreign-born (Tapestry) and les professions des Français (Libre Echange).
While these represent a normal use of written language for display and information, the purposes for which the students use them may be less authentic, partly because they are learning, say, the names for the clothes rather than deciding what to buy, i.e. a codebreaking rather than a decoding activity (Cook,, 2001b). The written language consists of noun phrases, proper names, prices etc rather than being 'full' sentences or having textual coherence – block language.
fill-in forms and charts
Students fill in copious amounts of information in invitations, diary planners: Daily Planner Monday Morning …… Afternoon …. Evening … … (Atlas) and forms: Carte Internationale d'Embarquement 1. M/Mme/Mlle ___________ Nom ______ … (Libre Echange). Mostly their response is a single word or phrase rather than a complete sentence or paragraph. This shades over into the innumerable charts for students to fill in, already mentioned. In Changes for instance the listening activities often rely on a chart such as: Listen to four people talking about the food they eat on different days of the week. Complete the chart.
In everyday life we do of course have to fill in such forms from time to time but mostly this is an unavoidable chore. Sorting information into columns manually is rare – would anyone not use their computer for this now? The language is fragments and isolated words. Most of the charts and forms have no outcome other than providing material for a teaching exercise.
sentence completion
Students fill in blanks in sentences before saying them aloud: Et …… laitues, il y a … … laitues (Libre Echange), or they construct sentences from jumbled words: Rearrange the words to make questions and answers and then practice them: you/where/live do? ... (Atlas). Sometimes the missing item is a word: Ana is Jonathan's ___ (Changes), sometimes a longer phrase Sure. ___ Campus Center Walk ___ the Campus Center (Tapestry).
In a sense this exploits the permanency of written language so that the whole sentence can be present simultaneously, again using written language in a way seldom encountered outside a classroom.
making up sentences
Students are given questions and answers in a jumbled order and have to pair them appropriately, for example My perfect weekend as seen by Stephen from Leeds and Paula from Nottingham (Reward). Sentences are also constructed from jumbled words: I dessert she want any steak don't fries they coffee some he wants (Atlas). A list of words provides a skeleton for retelling a story the students have heard: accident street break down capital cost … (Reward). Or indeed a sentence can be directly called for: Write an opening sentence about what happened to Bob (Reward).
This too is a purely pedagogical use of written language, inconceivable outside a classroom, relying similarly on reading aloud.
d) written texts
Most course-books also present written language through continuous texts longer than a single sentence.
short information texts
Cultural information is often presented through short texts: La France au Quotidien: les palmarès des villes françaises … (Libre Echange) or magazine-style written interviews: Intervistiamo una nota stilista italiana … (Ci Siamo). Short texts are often used as a basis for exercises, typically about 50 words long: biographies – Betty aged 95 from Nottingham (Changes), – first person accounts – Sun Hee Shi talks about her birthday (Changes) – or factual accounts – Amazing Animals (Did you know the kangaroo can't walk at all?) (Changes). Longer texts around 400 words are found on Superman (Tapestry 1), and The Day of the Dead (a Mexican festival rather than a 50s horror film) (Reward). Only Reward features a real short story, by Roald Dahl, and extracts from books such as Paul Theroux's The Kingdom by the Sea.
These short texts resemble pieces of travel guides or children's textbooks. Little of our everyday reading consists of passages of this type (particularly when they are pretexts for arranging information in columns or completing sentences).
letters
Specimens of letters occasionally form a basis for the students' own writing, for instance refusing an invitation (Atlas) or telling a friend about Italian pastimes (Ci Siamo).
Continuous written texts other than scripts of speech or instructions for exercises are in fact sparse in some course-books, though only Atlas appears to shun them apart from a few notices and scraps of newspapers. Virtually all the courses use short texts whose similarity to anything outside the coursebook is minimal; they do not have the display or structure of newspapers, say, but are straight pieces of information to use for teaching exercises.
Let us then sum up the conclusions about course-books reached in this section:
written scripts are primarily a way of teaching spoken language by providing a permanent record, not of teaching written language.
written language is often a device for explaining, giving instructions etc, i.e. a kind of meta-language of teaching, rather than a way-in to writing itself.
written language within teaching activities is mostly a pretext for spoken exercises, involving uses of language not encountered outside textbooks.
texts are mostly restricted to short quasi-factual biographies etc with some longer texts about 'interesting facts', again seldom recognisable as text types that would occur outside a teaching context.
These features seem typical of the course-books. Though they may be interpreted differently by different teachers, there is no reason to think that the average beginning student will encounter a totally different range of written language than that represented by these coursebooks.
3 The Nature of Written Language
The next step is to review some of the fundamentals of written language, dealt with at greater length in Cook, (2001a, to appear); section 4 will develop the teaching implications.
a) the two routes model
The 'standard' model of reading aloud relates letters to sounds via two mental routes (e.g. Paap et al, 1992). One 'sound-based' route uses correspondence rules between speech sounds and written forms: an English reader sees <tack> and consults mental rules that <t> corresponds to /t/, <a> to /¾ /, and <ck> to /k/. This route relies on rules for converting letters to sounds and vice versa – ‘assembling phonology from a word’s component letters’ (Katz & Frost, 1992, p.71).
The other 'visual' route utilises direct
links between written forms and meanings. corresponds in Chinese to the meaning 'person' regardless of its different
spoken forms in the various Chinese dialects. The English reader sees
<colonel> and consults a mental list of words to establish that it
corresponds to /k‘ nlÛ /. This route enables reading to be silent so that
readers can process written language at speeds much faster than they can read
aloud.
These two overall routes not only distinguish the writing systems used in different languages, but also operate within an individual user of a single language. Frequent English words are accessed as visual instances (Seidenberg, 1992); <the> for instance is perceived as a whole word rather than converted into sounds letter by letter; if you ask native speakers to cross out <e>s in a passage of English, the <e> in <the> is invisible (Cook,, to appear).
So written language involves not just the conversion of sounds into writing and vice versa but also direct connections to meaning. The assumption that sounds connect only to letters is a product of our own cultural bias. An L2 student who is moving from Chinese to English has different problems with written language from a student who is going from English to Spanish. L2 learners transfer their preferred route to the new language; thus Chinese learners of Japanese differ from English learners in being more visually oriented (Chikamatsu, 1996; Holm & Dodd, 1996); advanced Chinese learners on the other hand remain slower at reading English than Spanish learners (Haynes & Carr, 1990). Literate students bring to L2 learning the ideas about language created by the writing system of their first language, for good or for ill.
Course-books assume that written language simply provides a transcription of speech. This neglects the differences between languages, in particular the difficulty in acquiring a script of a different type, say English speakers learning Japanese or Chinese speakers learning Spanish. A single language of one overall type also contains elements of both systems: English is not purely sound-based, Chinese not purely meaning-based. A language user relies on both the sounds route and the visual route, to a greater or lesser degree depending on the language concerned and on word frequency.
The implications of the two routes are that teaching needs to acknowledge them rather than ignore them. The coursebooks do not deal overtly with the two routes. Students from different overall L1 writing systems are unprepared for the new system they are encountering, leading to persistent problems for Chinese students. The students, and probably the teachers, have no awareness that the two routes actually exist. Implicitly this reduces the teaching of English writing to apologies for its lack of system. Advanced L2 students still write phisical, neccessary and new (knew) because they have not been taught to treat some words as individual visual items (mistakes come from The 1400 corpus (Cook,, on-line)), and feever, persent and colledge because they believe writing is the reflection of speech. Some attention to both routes can do nothing but help.
b) the characteristics of written language
Here we will list some of the properties of written language briefly; other lists can be found in Brookes & Grundy (1998), Bygate (1998) and Ur (1996).
i) linguistic characteristics
vocabulary. Some words exist primarily in the written form,
say notices on road verges No parking on the greensward or on train
doors Alight other side.
grammar.
punctuation.
distinctive elements of writing.
spelling rules.
These linguistic characteristics of writing have been ignored in teaching. The written language of course-books does not reflect the standard properties of lexical density nor a proper range of punctuation: how could it when so much is sentence fragments? Far from being more contextualised, the written language of teaching is a limbo world of charts, lists, short texts, explanations and instructions.
This bias might be excusable if specific attention were paid to the actual rules of writing. Yet nowhere in the coursebooks can one find discussion or presentation, say, of spelling issues. If students were taught basic rules of English, they would not make mistakes such as usefull, ocured and dissappointment (rules of consonant doubling – 18% of the corpus) or studys, alwais or joyning (rules of <i/y> alternation); if they had been warned about transferring elements of their own L1 pronunciation through sound-letter rules, Japanese might not write grobal and literery (literally), Greeks Grade Britain and Gambridge, Arabic speakers punishement and subejects.
ii) overall characteristics
permanency of writing. Speech is fleeting: writing permanent. A written letter is present as long as, or as often as, we want to look at it; a spoken phoneme is gone as soon as it has been uttered and can only be checked against our internal memories or via an electronic recording. Speech is used for instant warnings Fire!, writing for permanent notices <KEEP OUT>. 'To write is indeed the only way of keeping or recapturing speech since speech denies itself as it gives itself' (Derrida, 1992). Writing is a memory substitute both for the society through written constitutions, history books, contracts, etc and for the individual through diaries, shopping lists, lecture notes and so on.
first and final drafts.
These characteristics are seldom seen in the course-books. Students may employ learning strategies based on the written language, whether making notes for future reference, keeping lists of vocabulary, or consulting dictionaries. Students, like L1 children, may need practice in developing a critical approach to successive drafts of their essays, letters, or indeed theses. A written sentence can always be improved; a spoken one cannot. All of these essential attributes of writing are relatively unexploited in the course-books.
iii) functional characteristics
Inevitably the differences between the two media have led to speech and writing being used for different purposes.
status
social roles.
Students should be exposed to the appropriate functions for both types of language, spoken language for the less formal, written language for the more formal, according to their particular needs. Some students may need informal roles – 'Hi, I'm Jim' – some more formal – 'Dr Livingstone, I presume'. The emphasis on informal or tourist conversation in beginners teaching has often not fitted the formal nature of the students' real-life encounters, let alone the written language they may need. They may require informal written language to use chat-lines and e-pal systems as well as the extra formality of many types of writing compared to speaking.
c) the literate L2 learner
Becoming literate in itself has effects on the individual:
literacy and cognition
literacy and the spoken language.
An L2 learner who already knows how to read thinks in different ways from a pre-literate child. Literate L2 learners have already established the two routes in their minds; they inevitably relate the new language to writing in a way that the non-literate do not; they are largely dependent on the type of writing system they have learnt in their first language. The changed mind of the L2 learner needs to be accommodated in teaching rather than ignored. The literate L2 learner cannot enter into language acquisition in the same way as the pre-literate native child. I can remember being surprised by the students in a strictly audiolingual class who were surreptitiously writing down the dialogues they heard under their desks. Pretending that students are not fully equipped with a knowledge of writing ignores the changes in the minds of literate L2 users and the powerful tool for learning provided by writing.
4. Possible reasons for emphasising spoken language
Perhaps, however teachers had good reason for assuming that the written language was just an adjunct to the spoken language. Let us then try to examine some of the reasons put forward for the priority of speech.
i) the argument from L1 learning
One overall justification is how children acquire their first language, just as it is for many nineteenth century teaching principles (Howatt, 1984; Banathy & Sawyer, 1969). As Harmer (1998, p.53) puts it, ‘Because many people acquire languages by hearing them first, many teachers prefer to expose students to the spoken form first’. If the only successful method of acquiring a language is that used by L1 children, the classroom should recreate the characteristics of the first language as closely as possible. Teaching cannot depend on things which are unavailable to L1 children, whether the ability to write, to understand grammar or to use another language.
This reflects more common-sense than theory or research. If the argument were true, no research would be needed into L2 acquisition or indeed into language teaching, since teaching needs simply to replicate the conditions of L1 acquisition. But L2 learning is intrinsically different from L1 learning because the L2 learner already knows how language works (Halliday, 1975) and how to impute mental intentions to other speakers (Tomasello, 1999). At best it might be said that in some respects, L2 learning is like L1 acquisition, in others it is not. The multi-competence viewpoint, however, denies that the comparison is proper since the L2 user has a complete system of two languages, not two separate systems (Cook,, 2002). There is no compelling reason why the accidental sequence of L1 acquisition should be relevant to literate L2 learners.
ii) arguments from linguistics
Most linguists and philosophers from Aristotle to Rousseau, from de Saussure to Bloomfield, have indeed believed in the primacy of speech, typified by Lyons (1968, p.38) 'the spoken language is primary and … writing is essentially a means of representing speech in another medium'. The arguments they present come down to the following:
speech comes before writing historically
Even if the historical argument were true, its use would involve ontogeny repeating phylogeny, on a par with a modern diet consisting of wild grain and meat we have slaughtered ourselves. There is no intrinsic reason why the classroom should parallel earlier societies, despite Taeschner's (1991) amazing language teaching method in which children recreate a primitive tribe. The invention of writing in itself changed the society that children and L2 users fit into. Human beings inherit the cultural achievements of their ancestors (Tomasello, 1999), partly through written language. Silent reading. for example, once a difficult and unusual skill, is now taken for granted in the primary school, thanks to the cultural innovation of word spaces in the 8th century AD (Saenger, 1997). L2 teaching should no more take pre-literate societies into account than physics teaching should cover medieval alchemy.
many languages lack a written form
many individuals cannot use written language
children automatically learn to speak but have to be taught how to read and write
iii) other teaching reasons
There are perhaps three supplementary reasons that can be mentioned.
students' needs.
students' demands.
interference.
The position of writing in teaching has then to be related to how writing functions in the mind of an L2 user, to the variations in writing systems and to the purposes for which the L2 user may put the L2. While some writers disparage writing as being of negligible importance (‘Nowadays most people actually do very little writing in day-to-day life’ (Scrivenor, 1994, p.156)), many people now spend much of their working day at a computer keyboard and, since the decline of the secretary, spend a high proportion of their time receiving and composing letters, e-mails and text messages. English language education for non-English-speaking students is a continual series of hurdles of written essays, projects and examinations.
5. A proper role for written language
We can now draw the argument together to make some conclusions about the current and potential roles of written language in language teaching.
i) teaching uses written language as a support for spoken language
As we have seen, the major use of written language is to support the activities of the classroom. Written language has been seen as a teaching aid, as neutral as a blackboard or an overhead projector, not as having value for the students in their using and learning of language nor as itself presenting them with problems – say, recognition of words by students from L1s without word divisions, uses of capital letters, untaught in beginners' course-books, or indeed direction of writing whether left-to-right or right-to-left. The written language is the servant of teaching rather than having a role of its own; it is used as a tool, not as a type of language the students actually need.
Obviously teaching may subordinate written language to spoken language if it thinks this is in the students' interests. But this decision should be made after considering the distortions it imposes on the written language: is the price worth paying? It is also a matter of proportion; how much of the written language the students encounter is a prop for writing, how much writing for its own sake? Above all are the students' goals best served by sacrificing writing to speaking? While most students may indeed need spoken language, this hardly prepares them for the reading and writing tasks of everyday life, whether looking at the newspaper, reading a novel or writing an e-mail.
ii) the presentation of written language significantly distorts the normal properties of writing
Time and again the previous discussion questioned whether typical teaching uses of written language existed outside the classroom. Opening one of these coursebooks at random, you see a page consisting of layouts of sentence fragments, lists of words, fill-in charts, etc. A typical page of Atlas, say page 57, has four exercises: exercise 1 is drawing lines between questions and answers; exercise 2 making up sentences from groups of words; exercise 3 filling in the blanks; exercise 4 writing 'a lot of' or 'enough' in the blanks in sentences. This represents an almost unique form of print, different from most books, magazines, newspapers or other everyday writing. The only similarities are with commercial publications for selling goods, say catalogues or travel agent's brochures, and with official forms for Income Tax returns etc. In our daily lives we seldom encounter lists of words that have to be sorted into groups, sentences with missing bits, written questions on our daily conversation, and so on.
Nor is the language that the students have to write any less fragmentary. Students fill in bits of noun phrases on forms; they complete sentences with words or phrases; they make graphic marks on paper. They seldom write a complete sentence, let alone a complete text; they rarely make up lists for themselves relevant to their learning or interests. The emphasis on speech has led to a misuse of writing. The teaching of the spoken language rarely distorts its nature to the same extent; at best the students use full sentences of the language in conversational turns, at worst appropriate sentence fragment answers to questions.
The solution for language teaching is then to teach the properties of the written language. One step in this direction is to use authentic written language, that is to say examples of writing produced for purposes other than language teaching, whether by native speakers or non-native speakers. At least then the teacher has the protection of knowing that the written language has its natural qualities. Another step is to systematically teach the relevant properties of language whether writing system, spelling or other conventions. As well as the use of written language for teaching, the students should at least encounter writing used as a part of human experience. None of this would be extraordinary when applied to speech; it is taken for granted that spoken language is presented as speech functions, conversations, interactions, sometimes authentic, sometimes simulated at a level the student can cope with. Only the bias towards speech has stopped the same criteria being applied to writing.
iii) the nature of the tasks that use written language
A vogue word in 1990s language teaching was task-based learning; indeed Atlas claims specifically to use 'task-based methodology'. Jane Willis (1996) lists six main type of task: listing, ordering and sorting, comparing, problem solving, sharing personal experience and creative. Atlas 1 (Nunan, 1995, teacher's book) uses ten types of task including predicting (for instance 'predicting what is to come on the learning process'), conversational patterns ('using expressions to start conversations and keep them going') and co-operating ('sharing ideas and learning with other students').
As we have seen, the tasks employ written language to foster speech. Take the sentence-invention exercise type we have often come across, typified by Tapestry: Make a sentence with each of these weather words. Practice saying the sentences to yourself. Then say the sentences with a partner. The sheer arbitrariness of sentence completion was something that the Reform Movement of the late nineteenth century attacked in previous methods: 'connected texts on worthwhile topics were clearly preferable to the pointless sentences of traditional grammar' (Howatt, 1984, 173). In its day the communicative teaching movement similarly reacted against the meaningless uncontextualised sentences of drills. Nevertheless sentence invention is a staple in nearly all the coursebooks. The lesson of the Reform Movements has not been applied to the teaching of written language. Spoken language in the classroom tries to be connected, relevant discourse; written language is by and large unconnected fragments acting as the impetus to exercises or as the source of explanations and instructions, largely true even of course-books specifically aimed at writing, such as Tapestry 1 Writing (Pike-Baky, 2000) which has a similar mixture of fill-in sentences, vocabulary lists and sentence fragments.
One unacknowledged element in many tasks is reading aloud. In some cases this is integral to the exercise, say fill-in sentences or substitution tables where the product is the student saying aloud what is written down. In other cases the exercise could not take place without some reading aloud, words in lists for instance. In so far as methodology books mention reading aloud, they warn against it: 'Do not ask students to read texts aloud' (Cross, 1992, p.80). Yet these coursebooks require students to read something aloud on nearly every page. While this is not quite the same as reading whole texts aloud, it is nonetheless converting written to spoken language. In some ways this fragmentary reading is worse than text reading since the fragments do not have the discourse coherence of complete texts, resulting inter alia in poor intonation. Nor do the coursebooks encourage the students to practice the skill of silent reading.
Though it goes outside the brief here, many of the tasks are essentially those used in the primary school – listing, classifying, mapping, and the like, hardly stretching adult intelligence and interests. The fact that they are carried out in a second language is .arbitrary; there is no logical reason why most of them have to be done through a second language.
6. conclusions
The aim here was not to disprove the priority of speech so much as to raise legitimate doubts about the unacknowledged neglect and distortion of writing in the early stages. Language teaching should not be restricted by an invisible straitjacket of assumptions unquestioned since the nineteenth century. If there are indeed powerful reasons for the devaluing of writing, they need to be put forward explicitly.
By neglecting written language, teaching is depriving students of an important element in their lives and minds in a way that other academic subjects do not; music may be pure sound but music teaching in the western tradition emphasises written notes. Putting written language in second place ignores its importance in the minds of literate people. It is short-sighted to make literate students do without its help. Kern (2000) points out that writing develops the ability to think, helps learners to acquire appropriate form/meaning relationships, allows them to develop their ideas, and encourages imaginative use of language. Above all, literate learners use written language as their record of what has happened, to be resorted to when needed, whether the vocabulary list they have made for themselves or the notes they have made in class.
The overall conclusions for language teaching are then twofold. Firstly it should try to curb unnatural practices in the written language. Even if the goal is eventual written language alone, this misuse will have a bad effect on the literate L2 learner. Secondly it should attempt to teach the distinctive elements of writing that are needed by any student who wants to use the second language for more than filling in forms. Written language is at the centre of many uses of language, particularly in the computer age and its characteristics are no more likely to be assimilated unaided than are those of the spoken language.
Footnote
Many helpful comments and criticisms have been made of earlier drafts by Tony Lilley, Ignazia Posadinu, Guy Cook, Martin Bygate and Brian Tomlinson.
References
Albrow, K.H. (1972). The English Writing System: notes towards a description. Harlow: Longman
Banathy, B.H. & Sawyer, J.O. (1969). The primacy of speech: an historical sketch. Modern Language Journal, 53, 537-44
Benz, C. & Dworak, M. (2000). Tapestry 1 Listening and Speaking. Boston: Heinle and Heinle
Brookes, A. & Grundy, P. (1998). Beginning to Write. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Bygate, M. (1998). Theoretical perspectives on speaking. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 18, 20-42
Carney, E. (1994). A Survey of English Spelling. London: Routledge
Chikamatsu, N. (1996). The effects of L1 orthography on L2 word recognition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18, 403-432
Chomsky, C. (1970). Reading, writing and phonology. Harvard Educational Review, 40, 2, 287-309
Chomsky, N. (1995). The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press
Coleman, A. (1929). The Teaching of Modern Languages in the United States. New York: American and Canadian Committees on Foreign Languages
Courtillon, J. & de Salins, G-D. (1995). Libre Echange. Paris: Hatier/Didier
Cook, V.J. (1989). The relevance of grammar to the applied linguistics of language teaching. Trinity College Dublin Occasional Papers, 22
Cook, V.J. (2001a). Knowledge of Writing. International Review of Applied Linguistics, 39, 1-18, 2001
Cook, V.J. (2001b). Second Language Learning and Language Teaching. London: Edward Arnold. Third edition
Cook, V.J. (2002). Background to the L2 user perspective. In Cook,, (Ed.). Portraits of the L2 User. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters
Cook, V.J. (on-line). The 1400 Sample of L2 Learners' Spelling Mistakes
Cook, V.J. (to appear). The English Writing System. London: Edward Arnold
Cross, D. (1992). A Practical Handbook of Language Teaching. Hemel Hempstead: Prentice-Hall
Cuban Ministry of Education (1999). Principios que rigen la ensen Í anza del ingles en la escuala media. Ministry of Education, Cuba
Derrida, J. (1992).
Acts of Literature, (ed. D. Attridge). New York: RoutledgeDerwing, B.L. (1992). Orthographic aspects of linguistic competence. In Downing, P., Lima, S.D. & Noonan, M. (Eds.). The Linguistics of Literacy. Amsterdam: Benjamins, 193–210
Goody, J. (2000). The Power of the Written Tradition. Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Institution Press
Greenall, S. (1994). Reward. Oxford: Heinemann
Guarnuccio, C. & Guarnuccio, E. (1997). Ci Siamo. Victoria: CIS Heinemann
Halliday, M.A.K. (1975), Learning How to Mean, London: Edward Arnold
Halliday, M.A.K. (1985). Spoken and Written Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Harmer, J. (1998). How to Teach English. Harlow: Longman
Haynes, M. & Carr, T.H. (1990). Writing system background and second language reading: a component skills analysis of English reading by native-speaking readers of Chinese. In Carr, T.H. & Levy, B.A. (Eds.). Reading and its Development: Component Skills Approaches. San Diego: Academic Press, 375-421
Holm, A. & Dodd, B. (1996). The effect of first written language on the acquisition of English literacy. Cognition, 59, 119-147
Householder, F.W. (1971). Linguistic Speculations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Howatt, A. (1984). A History of English Language Teaching. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Katz, L. & Frost, R. (1992). Reading in different orthographies: the orthographic depth hypothesis. In Frost, R. & Katz, L. (Eds.). Orthography, Phonology, Morphology and Meaning. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 67-84
Kern, R. (2000). Literacy and Language Teaching. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Krashen, S. (1993). The Power of Reading. Englewood Colorado: Libraries Unlimited Inc.
Kress, G. (2000). Early Spelling. London: Routledge
Lado, R. (1964). Language Teaching: A Scientific Approach. New York: McGraw-Hill
Luria, A. (1976). Cognitive Development: Its Cultural and Social Foundations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Lyons, J. (1968). Introduction to Theoretical Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Nunan, D. (1995). Atlas. Boston: Heinle and Heinle
Nunberg, G. (1990). Linguistics of Punctuation. Stanford: CSLI
Olson, D.R. (1996). Toward a psychology of literacy: on the relations between speech and writing. Cognition, 60, 83-104
Paap, K.R., Noel, R.W. & Johansen, L.S. (1992). Dual-route models of print to sound: red herrings and real horses. In Frost, R. & Katz, L. (Eds.). Orthography, Phonology, Morphology, and Meaning. Amsterdam; Elsevier, 293-318
Petersen, K.M., Reis, A., Askelöf, S., Castro-Caldas, A. & Ingvar, M. (2000). Language processing modulated by literacy: a network analysis of verbal repetition in literate and illiterate subjects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 3,
Pike-Baky, M. (2000). Tapestry 1 Writing. Boston: Heinle & Heinle
Richards J.C. (1998). Changes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Saenger, P. (1997). Space between Words. Stanford: Stanford University Press
Scrivenor, J. (1994). Learning Teaching. Oxford: Heinemann
Seely, C. & Romijn, E.K. (1998). TPR is more than Commands. 2nd edition, Berkeley CA: Command Perfomance Language Institute
Seidenberg, M.S. (1992). Beyond orthographic depth in reading: equitable division of labour. In Frost, R. & Katz, L. (Eds.). Orthography, Phonology, Morphology and Meaning. Amsterdam; Elsevier, 85-118
Soars L. &. Soars, J. (1993). Headway Elementary. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Stern, H.H. (1983). Fundamental Concepts of Language Teaching, Oxford: Oxford University Press
Sweet, H. (1899). The Practical Study of Language. London: Dent
Taeschner, T. (1991). A Developmental Psycholinguistic Approach to Second Language Teaching. New Jersey: Ablex
Terrell, T.D., Egasse, J. & Andrade, M. (1990). Dos Mundos: a Communicative Approach. New York: McGraw-Hill, 2nd edition
Tomasello, M. (1999). The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press
Treiman, R. (1993). Beginning to Spell. Oxford: Oxford University Press
UNESCO Statistic Yearbook (2000). http://unescostat.unesco.org/en/stats/stats0.htm
Ur, P. (1996). A Course in Language Teaching, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Valdman, A. (1966), Introduction. In Valdman, A. (Ed.). Trends in Language Teaching. New York: McGraw-Hill